Background: Failure to resist temptation may make people experience an action-control emotion of regret. We explored whether evoking regret after participants’ failure to attend physical exercise sessions would prompt them to undertake compensatory behaviors of different sorts.
Participants and procedure: Physically active men and women (N = 133) were invited to attend a 6-session fitness course. Using cluster randomization, participants were assigned either to the experimental group with a regret-evoking message after they failed to show up at a fitness session (n = 69), or the control group, which did not receive the regret-evoking message (n = 64). After missing any of the first five fitness sessions participants reported whether they had engaged in compensatory health behaviors during the preceding 24 hours. Data were collected at pre-test, post-test, and after each missed fitness session.
Results: Linear mixed-effects analysis showed no effects of the regret manipulation on compensatory behaviors such as physical activity, nutrition, or substance use avoidance. However, the manipulation resulted in lower levels of engagement in socializing behaviors.
Conclusions: The results suggest that people engage in compensatory behaviors after a lapse in physical exercise. Evoked regret may serve to a limited extent as a means to prompt compensatory behaviors.
REFERENCES(40)
1.
Abraham, C., & Sheeran, P. (2003). Acting on intentions: The role of anticipated regret. British Journal of Social Psychology, 42, 495–511. doi: 10.1348/014466603322595248.
Abraham, C., & Sheeran, P. (2004). Deciding to exercise: The role of anticipated regret. British Journal of Health Psychology, 9, 269–278. doi: 10.1348/135910704773891096.
Anderson, C. B., & Bulik, C. M. (2004). Gender differences in compensatory behaviours, weight and shape salience, and drive for thinness. Eating Behaviours, 5, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2003.07.001.
Baumeister, R. F., Stillwell, A. M., & Heatherton, T. F. (1994). Guilt: An interpersonal approach. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 243–267. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.115.2.243.
Baumeister, R. F., Stillwell, A. M., & Heatherton, T. F. (1995). Personal narratives about guilt: Role in action control and interpersonal relationships. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 17, 173–198. doi: 10.1080/01973533.1995.9646138.
Byrka, K., & Kaiser, F. G. (2013). Health performance of individuals within the Campbell paradigm. International Journal of Psychology, 48, 986–999. doi: 10.1080/00207594.2012.702215.
Carels, R. A., Hoffman, J., Collins, A., Raber, A. C., Cacciapaglia, H., & O’Brien, W. H. (2001). Ecological momentary assessment of temptation and lapse in dieting. Eating Behaviours, 2, 307–321. doi: 10.1016/S1471-0153(01)00037-X.
Conner, M., Conner, M., Sandberg, T., McMillan, B., & Higgins, A. (2006). Role of anticipated regret, intentions and intention stability in adolescent smoking initiation. British Journal of Health Psychology, 11, 85–101. doi: 10.1348/135910705X40997.
de Nooijer, J., Lechner, L., Candel, M., & de Vries, H. (2004). Short- and long-term effects of tailored information versus general information on determinants and intentions related to early detection of cancer. Preventive Medicine, 38, 694–703. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2003.12.016.
Ernsting, A., Schwarzer, R., Lippke, S., & Schneider, M. (2013). ‘I do not need a flu shot because I lead a healthy lifestyle’: Compensatory health beliefs make vaccination less likely. Journal of Health Psychology, 18, 825–836.
Gilovich, T., & Medvec, V. H. (1995). The experience of regret: What, when, and why. Psychological Review, 102, 379–395. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.102.2.379.
Giner-Sorolla, R. (2001). Guilty pleasures and grim necessities: Affective attitudes in dilemmas of self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 206–221. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.2.206.
Hayes, J., Schimel, J., Arndt, J., & Faucher, E. H. (2010). A theoretical and empirical review of the death-thought accessibility concept in terror management research. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 699–739. doi: 10.1037/a0020524.
Kaklamanou, D., Armitage, C. J., & Jones, C. R. (2013). A further look into compensatory health beliefs. British Journal of Health Psychology, 18, 139–154.
Kilpatrick, M., Hebert, E., & Bartholomew, J. (2005). College Students’ motivation for physical activity: Differentiating men’s and women’s motives for sport participation and exercise. Journal of American College Health, 54, 87–94. doi: 10.3200/JACH.54.2.87-94.
Knäuper, B., Rabiau, M., Cohen, O., & Patriciu, N. (2004). Compensatory health beliefs: scale development and psychometric properties. Psychology & Health, 19, 607–624. doi: 10.1080/0887044042000196737.
Kronick, I., Auerbach, R. P., Stich, C., & Knäuper, B. (2011). Compensatory beliefs and intentions contribute to the prediction of caloric intake in dieters. Appetite, 57, 435–438. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.05.306.
Miller, D. T., Turnbull, W., & McFarland, C. (1989). When a coincidence is suspicious: The role of mental simulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 581–589. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.57.4.581.
Miquelon, P., Knäuper, B., & Vallerand, R. J. (2012). Motivation and goal attainment. The role of compensatory beliefs. Appetite, 58, 608–615. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.12.025.
Nudelman, G., & Shiloh, S. (2015). Mapping health behaviors: constructing and validating a common sense taxonomy of health behaviors. Social Science & Medicine, 146, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2015.10.004.
O’Connor, R. C., & Armitage, C. J. (2003). Theory of planned behaviour and parasuicide: An exploratory study. Current Psychology, 22, 196–205. doi: 10.1007/s12144-003-1016-4.
Quisenberry, A. J., Eddy, C. R., Patterson, D. L., Franck, C. T., & Bickel, W. K. (2015). Regret expression and social learning increases delay to sexual gratification. PLoS One, 10, e0135977. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0135977.
Rabiau, M., Knäuper, B., & Miquelon, P. (2006). The eternal quest for optimal balance between maximizing pleasure and minimizing harm: The compensatory health beliefs model. British Journal of Health Psychology, 11, 139–153. doi: 10.1348/135910705X52237.
Rabiau, M. A., Knäuper, B., Nguyen, T. K., Sufrategui, M., & Polychronakos, C. (2009). Compensatory beliefs about glucose testing are associated with low adherence to treatment and poor metabolic control in adolescents with type 1 diabetes. Health Education Research, 24, 890–896. doi: 10.1093/her/cyp032.
Radtke, T., Scholz, U., Keller, R., & Hornung, R. (2012). Smoking is ok as long as I eat healthily: Compensatory Health Beliefs and their role for intentions and smoking within the Health Action Process Approach. Psychology & Health, 27, 91–107.
Richard, R., de Vries, N. K., & van der Pligt, J. (1998). Anticipated regret and precautionary sexual behaviour. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28, 1411–1428. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1998.tb01684.x.
Richard, R., van der Pligt, J., & de Vries, N. (1996). Anticipated affect and behavioural choice. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 18, 111–129. doi: 10.1207/s15324834basp1802_1.
Sandberg, T., & Conner, M. (2008). Anticipated regret as an additional predictor in the theory of planned behaviour: A meta-analysis. British Journal of Social Psychology, 47, 589–606. doi: 10.1348/014466607X258704.
Sheeran, P., & Orbell, S. (1999). Augmenting the theory of planned behaviour: Roles for anticipated regret and descriptive norms. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 29, 2107–2142. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1999.tb02298.x.
Sherman, S. J. (1980). On the self-erasing nature of errors of prediction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 211–221. doi: 10.1177/0022022106297301.
Thompson, E. R. (2007). Development and validation of an internationally reliable short-form of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS). Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38, 227–242. doi: 10.1177/0022022106297301.
We process personal data collected when visiting the website. The function of obtaining information about users and their behavior is carried out by voluntarily entered information in forms and saving cookies in end devices. Data, including cookies, are used to provide services, improve the user experience and to analyze the traffic in accordance with the Privacy policy. Data are also collected and processed by Google Analytics tool (more).
You can change cookies settings in your browser. Restricted use of cookies in the browser configuration may affect some functionalities of the website.